Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 15 de 15
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Autism ; : 13623613231216052, 2023 Dec 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38153207

RESUMO

LAY ABSTRACT: People with autism spectrum conditions (ASC) have difficulties imagining events, which might result from difficulty mentally generating and maintaining a coherent spatial scene. This study compared this scene construction ability between autistic (N = 55) and neurotypical (N = 63) adults. Results showed that scene construction was diminished in autistic compared to neurotypical participants, and participants with fewer autistic traits had better scene construction ability. ASC diagnosis did not influence the frequency of mentions of the self or of sensory experiences. Exploratory analysis suggests that scene construction ability is associated with the ability to understand our own and other people's mental states, and that these individual-level preferences/cognitive styles can overrule typical group-level characteristics.

2.
Aggress Behav ; 49(2): 110-126, 2023 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36332082

RESUMO

Bystanders' helping interventions in bias-based bullying are rare, although they have the potential to intervene on behalf of the victim and quickly stop the aggression. Two studies tested, experimentally, the impact of adolescents' imagined (Study 1, N = 113, Mage = 16.17) and extended contact experiences (Study 2, N = 174, Mage = 15.79) on assertive bystanders' behavioral intentions in the context of homophobic bullying, an under-researched but highly detrimental behavior that emerges mainly during early adolescence. Potential mediators (empathic concern, social contagion concerns, and masculinity/femininity threat) were also examined. Results showed that female younger participants revealed more behavioral intentions to help victims of homophobic bullying when asked to imagine an interaction with an outgroup member (Study 1). Younger participants revealed less masculinity/femininity threat in the positive extended contact condition, and female participants revealed less empathic concern in the negative extended contact condition (Study 2). Overall, these findings identify specific conditions (e.g., younger females) where indirect contact interventions (i.e., extended and imagined) are likely to have a stronger impact. Age and sex differences were found to illustrate how adolescents vary in their behavioral intentions, empathic concern, and threat; and also highlight the need to further examine age and sex differences regarding responses to homophobic bullying episodes.


Assuntos
Bullying , Vítimas de Crime , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adolescente , Intenção , Agressão , Empatia , Masculinidade
3.
J Appl Psychol ; 106(12): 1821-1833, 2021 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34968092

RESUMO

Managers and customers often expect individuals to be "ideal workers" devoted entirely to work, and this devotion is typically displayed through being available to work at any time, on any day (Reid, 2015). During the COVID-19 pandemic, many individuals in lower-paid, customer-facing jobs were expected to not only be available but also to take on physical risk. However, the ideal worker literature has paid relatively little attention to how risk relates to ideal worker expectations, reflecting in part the extant literature's focus on professionals who face relatively little physical and financial uncertainty. In this article, we draw upon the experiences of nonprofessional "gig" workers (TaskRabbit workers) to examine how they manage customers' ideal worker expectations-including risk-using data from interviews (n = 49), postings from online worker forums social media, and offical company communications. We show how these workers engage in different tactics to manage risk in response to customers' expectations, including two tactics-covering and withdrawing-that have not been discussed in prior ideal worker literature. In doing so, we expand scholarly understanding by showing how concerns about risk shape workers' responses to ideal worker expectations, particularly in customer-facing service work outside of traditional organizations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
COVID-19 , Pandemias , Comércio , Humanos , Motivação , SARS-CoV-2
4.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 199: 104892, 2020 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32682100

RESUMO

Differences between children's and parents' implicit and explicit gender stereotypes were investigated in two experiments. For the first time, the visual world paradigm compared parents' and 7-8-year-old children's looking preferences toward masculine- and feminine-typed objects stereotypically associated with a story character's gender. In Experiment 1 participants listened to sentences that included a verb that inferred intentional action with an object (e.g., "Lilly/Alexander will play with the toy"), and in Experiment 2 the verb was replaced with a neutral verb (e.g., "Lilly/Alexander will trip over the toy"). A questionnaire assessed participants' explicit gender stereotype endorsement (and knowledge [Experiment 2]) of children's toys. Results revealed that parents and children displayed similar implicit stereotypes, but different explicit stereotypes, to one another. In Experiment 1, both children and parents displayed looking preferences toward the masculine-typed object when the story character was male and looking preferences toward the feminine-typed object when the character was female. No gender effects were found with a neutral verb in Experiment 2, reinforcing the impact of gender stereotypes on implicit processing and showing that the effects are not simply driven by gender stereotypic name-object associations. In the explicit measure, parents did not endorse the gender stereotypes related to toys but rather appeared to be egalitarian, whereas children's responses were gender stereotypic.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Pais/psicologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Jogos e Brinquedos/psicologia , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Criança , Tecnologia de Rastreamento Ocular , Feminino , Identidade de Gênero , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Estereotipagem , Reino Unido
5.
Sex Roles ; 79(5): 314-328, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30147223

RESUMO

Extensive evidence has documented the gender stereotypic content of children's media, and media is recognized as an important socializing agent for young children. Yet, the precise impact of children's media on the endorsement of gender-typed attitudes and behaviors has received less scholarly attention. We investigated the impact of stereotypic and counter-stereotypic peers pictured in children's magazines on children's gender flexibility around toy play and preferences, playmate choice, and social exclusion behavior (n = 82, age 4-7 years-old). British children were randomly assigned to view a picture of a peer-age boy and girl in a magazine playing with either a gender stereotypic or counter-stereotypic toy. In the stereotypic condition, the pictured girl was shown with a toy pony and the pictured boy was shown with a toy car; these toys were reversed in the counter-stereotypic condition. Results revealed significantly greater gender flexibility around toy play and playmate choices among children in the counter-stereotypic condition compared to the stereotypic condition, and boys in the stereotypic condition were more accepting of gender-based exclusion than were girls. However, there was no difference in children's own toy preferences between the stereotypic and counter-stereotypic condition, with children preferring more gender-typed toys overall. Implications of the findings for media, education, and parenting practices are discussed, and the potential for counter-stereotypic media portrayals of toy play to shape the gender socialization of young children is explored.

6.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 33(4): 419-33, 2015 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26058823

RESUMO

A developmental intergroup approach was taken to examine the development of prosocial bystander intentions among children and adolescents. Participants as bystanders (N = 260) aged 8-10 and 13-15 years were presented with scenarios of direct aggression between individuals from different social groups (i.e., intergroup verbal aggression). These situations involved either an ingroup aggressor and an outgroup victim or an outgroup aggressor and an ingroup victim. This study focussed on the role of intergroup factors (group membership, ingroup identification, group norms, and social-moral reasoning) in the development of prosocial bystander intentions. Findings showed that prosocial bystander intentions declined with age. This effect was partially mediated by the ingroup norm to intervene and perceived severity of the verbal aggression. However, a moderated mediation analysis showed that only when the victim was an ingroup member and the aggressor an outgroup member did participants become more likely with age to report prosocial bystander intentions due to increased ingroup identification. Results also showed that younger children focussed on moral concerns and adolescents focussed more on psychological concerns when reasoning about their bystander intention. These novel findings help explain the developmental decline in prosocial bystander intentions from middle childhood into early adolescence when observing direct intergroup aggression.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Intenção , Princípios Morais , Comportamento Social , Percepção Social , Pensamento/fisiologia , Comportamento Verbal/fisiologia , Adolescente , Criança , Feminino , Processos Grupais , Humanos , Masculino
7.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 33(3): 277-94, 2015 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25773274

RESUMO

When will children decide to help outgroup peers? We examined how intergroup competition, social perspective taking (SPT), and empathy influence children's (5-10 years, N = 287) prosocial intentions towards outgroup members. Study 1 showed that, in a minimal group situation, prosociality was lower in an intergroup competitive than in a non-competitive or interpersonal context. Study 2 revealed that, in a real groups situation involving intergroup competition, prosociality was associated with higher empathy and lower competitive motivation. In a subsequent non-competitive context, there were age differences in the impact of SPT and competitive motivation. With age, relationships strengthened between SPT and prosociality (positively) and between competitiveness and prosociality (negatively). Among older children, there was a carry-over effect whereby feelings of intergroup competitiveness aroused by the intergroup competitive context suppressed outgroup prosociality in the following non-competitive context. Theoretical and practical implications for improving children's intergroup relationships are discussed.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Comportamento Competitivo , Processos Grupais , Comportamento de Ajuda , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Empatia , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Intenção , Masculino , Grupo Associado , Teoria da Mente
8.
Dev Psychol ; 50(1): 258-70, 2014 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23544855

RESUMO

Research with adults has demonstrated a "black sheep effect" (BSE) whereby, relative to evaluations of normative group members, ingroup deviants are derogated more than outgroup deviants. The developmental subjective group dynamics (DSGD) model holds that the BSE should develop during middle childhood when children apply wider social norms. Three hundred and thirty-eight children who were between 5 and 12 years old judged a normative (socially desirable) and a deviant (socially undesirable) member from an ingroup or an outgroup school. Results confirmed a developmental increase in the BSE, the first time this has been demonstrated. Children's own evaluations of group members were mediated by their expectations about ingroup peers' evaluations. In line with DSGD and social domain theories, with age, children's explanations of peer evaluations for ingroup deviance focused relatively more on loyalty. Practical and theoretical implications for peer inclusion and exclusion are discussed.


Assuntos
Relações Interpessoais , Grupo Associado , Identificação Social , Percepção Social , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Valores de Referência , Autoimagem , Comportamento Social
9.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 39(12): 1656-67, 2013 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24052085

RESUMO

A 1-year longitudinal study with three testing points was conducted with 215 British Asian children aged 5 to 11 years to test hypotheses from Berry's acculturation framework. Using age-appropriate measures of acculturation attitudes and psychosocial outcomes, it was found that (a) children generally favored an "integrationist" attitude, and this was more pronounced among older (8-10 years) than in younger (5-7 years) children and (b) temporal changes in social self-esteem and peer acceptance were associated with different acculturation attitudes held initially, as shown by latent growth curve analyses. However, a supplementary time-lagged regression analysis revealed that children's earlier "integrationist" attitudes may be associated with more emotional symptoms (based on teachers' ratings) 6 months later. The implications of these different outcomes of children's acculturation attitudes are discussed.


Assuntos
Aculturação , Atitude , Ajustamento Social , Adaptação Psicológica , Fatores Etários , Ásia Ocidental , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Grupo Associado , Distância Psicológica , População Branca
10.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 30(Pt 2): 283-302, 2012 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22550949

RESUMO

This research examined whether peer relationships amongst ethnic minority status children reflect the social groups to which children belong and the degree to which they identify with these groups. A longitudinal study was conducted to investigate the influence of group identities (i.e., ethnic and national) on children's perceived peer acceptance and preference for same-ethnic friendships. Measures of ethnic and English identification, perceived peer acceptance, and friendship choice were administered to 207 south-Asian English children, aged between 5 and 11, at two time points 6 months apart. In line with predictions, longitudinal analysis showed that bicultural identification (i.e., higher ethnic and English identity) was related to higher perceived peer acceptance and less preference for same-ethnic friendships. Importantly, as hypothesized, this finding was limited to the older children with more advanced social-cognitive abilities. The results suggest that older children who adopted a bicultural identity were able to strategically 'flag' their multiple group identities, within their multicultural peer groups, to obtain acceptance amongst the maximum number of peers and show less preference for same-ethnic friendships. This study extends previous peer relations research, which has typically focused on individual social deficits or classroom norms, by showing that group identities influence peer relationships amongst ethnic minority status children.


Assuntos
Etnicidade/psicologia , Amigos/psicologia , Grupos Minoritários/psicologia , Grupo Associado , Comportamento Social , Percepção Social , Ásia/etnologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Distância Psicológica , Autoimagem , Identificação Social , Reino Unido , População Branca/psicologia
11.
An. psicol ; 27(3): 708-717, oct.-dic. 2011. tab
Artigo em Inglês | IBECS | ID: ibc-94309

RESUMO

En esta investigación se ha evaluado un nuevo concepto en psicología social, denominado "contacto imaginado‟, en niños pequeños (n = 123, de 5 a 10 años). A la mitad de la muestra se les pidió que se imaginaran a sí mismos interactuando con un niño con una discapacidad física; la otra mitad no participó en esta actividad (grupo control). En comparación con el grupo control, los niños en la condición "contacto imaginado‟ mostraron posteriormente una reducción del sesgo intergrupal cuando se evaluaban sus actitudes generales y sus valoraciones de cordialidad y aptitudes. El "contacto imaginado‟ también se asoció con un mayor deseo de desarrollar conductas de amistad hacia el niño con discapacidad, pero solo en los participantes de 5 -6 años. Esto apoya parcialmente nuestra hipótesis de que los niños pequeños, quizá como resultado de una falta de experiencia con el exogrupo, se benefician en mayor medida del contacto imaginado. Se discuten las implicaciones para el desarrollo de las actitudes hacia las personas con discapacidad, la teoría del contacto imaginado y el desarrollo de técnicas de reducción de los prejuicios en el aula basadas en el contacto imaginado (AU)


The current research tested a recent development in social psychology, namely "imagined contact‟, among young children (n = 123, 5 to 10 years). Children imagined interacting with a physically disabled child, or did not take part in this activity (the control group). Compared with the control group, children who engaged in "imagined contact‟ subsequently showed reduced intergroup bias in their general attitude and ratings of warmth and competence. Imagined contact also led to more positive intended friendship behavior towards the disabled, but only among 5 – 6 year olds. This provides partial support for our hypothesis that younger children, perhaps as a result of their lack of out-group experience, are more likely to benefit from imagined contact. Implications for the development of attitudes towards the disabled, imagined contact theory and the development of classroom-based prejudice-reduction techniques based on imagined contact are discussed (AU)


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Criança , Crianças com Deficiência/educação , Crianças com Deficiência/psicologia , Psicologia Social/ética , Imaginação/ética , Crianças com Deficiência/reabilitação , Crianças com Deficiência/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicologia Social/educação , Psicologia Social/métodos , Psicologia Social/estatística & dados numéricos , Imaginação/classificação , Imaginação/fisiologia
12.
Dev Psychol ; 43(1): 134-48, 2007 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17201514

RESUMO

To test social and cognitive variables that may affect the development of subjective group dynamics, the authors had 224 children between the ages of 5 and 12 years evaluate an in-group and an out-group and normative and deviant in-group members under conditions of high or low accountability to in-group peers. In-group bias and relative favorability to normative versus deviant in-group members (differential evaluation) increased when children were accountable to peers and as a function of perceptions of peer group acceptance of these members (differential inclusion). These effects were significantly larger among older children. Multiple classification ability was unrelated to judgments of group members. This study shows that the development of subjective group dynamics involves an increase in sensitivity to the normative aspects of the intergroup context.


Assuntos
Processos Grupais , Grupo Associado , Identificação Social , Responsabilidade Social , Fatores Etários , Conscientização , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Masculino , Teoria da Construção Pessoal , Conformidade Social , Desejabilidade Social , Percepção Social
13.
Child Dev ; 77(5): 1208-19, 2006.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16999793

RESUMO

The present research evaluated an intervention, derived from the "extended contact hypothesis," which aimed to change children's intergroup attitudes toward refugees. The study (n=253) tested 3 models of extended contact among 5- to 11-year-old children: dual identity, common ingroup identity, and decategorization. Children read friendship stories based upon these models featuring in- and outgroup members. Outgroup attitudes were significantly more positive in the extended contact conditions, compared with the control, and this was mediated by "inclusion of other in self." The dual identity intervention was the most effective extended contact model at improving outgroup attitudes. The effect of condition on outgroup intended behavior was moderated by subgroup identity. Implications for theoretically based prejudice-reduction interventions among children are discussed.


Assuntos
Atitude , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Relações Interpessoais , Refugiados/psicologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Processos Grupais , Humanos , Masculino , Preconceito , Identificação Social
14.
Child Dev ; 76(2): 451-66, 2005.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15784093

RESUMO

Two studies examined whether social norms and children's concern for self-presentation affect their intergroup attitudes. Study 1 examined racial intergroup attitudes and normative beliefs among children aged 6 to 16 years (n=155). Accountability (i.e., public self-focus) was experimentally manipulated, and intergroup attitudes were assessed using explicit and implicit measures. Study 2 (n = 134) replicated Study 1, focusing on national intergroup attitudes. Both studies showed that children below 10 years old were externally motivated to inhibit their in-group bias under high public self-focus. Older children were internally motivated to suppress their bias as they showed implicit but not explicit bias. Study 1, in contrast to Study 2, showed that children with low norm internalization suppressed their out-group prejudice under high public self-focus.


Assuntos
Atitude , Relações Interpessoais , Autoimagem , Comportamento Social , Adolescente , Criança , Cultura , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Preconceito , Tempo de Reação , Estereotipagem , Gravação de Videoteipe
15.
Child Dev ; 74(6): 1840-56, 2003.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14669899

RESUMO

A developmental model of subjective group dynamics suggests that social identity is sustained first by intergroup biases and later by intragroup biases. In this study 476 English children 5 to 11 years old evaluated the English and German soccer teams, and judged in-group or out-group members whose attitudes toward the teams was normative versus antinormative. Children of all ages expressed intergroup bias. Differential evaluation against in-group deviants and in favor of out-group deviants strengthened with age. Understanding of targets' relative acceptability (differential inclusion) among in-group and out-group members mediated the effects of age and intergroup bias on intragroup bias. Identification with the in-group moderated the effects only among older children.


Assuntos
Atitude , Julgamento , Conformidade Social , Identificação Social , Percepção Social , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Desenvolvimento da Personalidade , Preconceito , Futebol/psicologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...